Does a restaurant providing the calorie counts of food items influence your decision making Why or why not?

After years of delays and setbacks, a mandate that all chain restaurants with 20 or more locations nationwide provide calorie information on their menus goes into effect Monday. As a result, fast food loyalists and chain restaurant patrons across the country will come face-to-face with the calorie counts for their favorite menu items.

Policymakers hope that calorie labeling will help consumers forego higher-calorie foods in favor of lower-calorie alternatives. This goal is especially important given that more than two-thirds of Americans are either overweight or obese and we spend close to $150 billion a year on the medical costs of obesity.

Detractors of calorie labeling legislation rightfully point out that most research studies have thus far found little to no effect of calorie information on how many calories consumers are ordering. Not surprisingly, this has led many commentators to call calorie labeling a “failed” policy.

advertisement

Not so fast. Our research, which is forthcoming in the Journal of Consumer Psychology, suggests that we should not dismiss calorie labeling just yet, and that a trivially simple intervention can considerably increase the impact of calorie labels on consumers’ food orders.

This intervention was borne from a simple observation we made when visiting restaurants that already display calorie counts on their menus. This information is almost always presented to the right of the menu items’ names, like this: House Special Cheeseburger — 870 Calories. Because Americans read from left to right, this means the calorie information is read after the food item’s name.

advertisement

Drawing on decades of psychological research showing that the first piece of information one reads usually receives disproportionate weight in subsequent decisions, we predicted that calorie information presented this way had little chance to affect food orders. The intuition is that a consumer may already be leaning toward a cheeseburger before getting a chance to consider the calorie information, thus minimizing its effect on what he or she orders.

We tested this by moving the calorie information from the right of the menu item to the left, like this: 870 Calories — House Special Cheeseburger. The effect of this simple adjustment was dramatic.

In one study at a casual chain restaurant on an American college campus, we found that diners who chose from a menu with calorie information displayed to the left of the food items ordered meals with 28.4 percent fewer calories compared to diners who chose from a menu without calorie information. By comparison, diners who chose from a menu with calorie information to the right of each menu item ordered meals with only 5.4 percent fewer calories.

We were able to test whether this effect was really about the order in which consumers read the calorie information in a follow-up study with Hebrew-speaking Israelis, who read from right to left. For them, placing calorie information earlier — on the right — should have greater impact. And it did: They ordered fewer calories when choosing from a menu with calorie information to the right of menu items, rather than the left. This follow-up study provided solid evidence that the order in which calorie information is presented and read has a powerful effect on whether consumers use it to make lower-calorie food choices.

As the nationwide rollout of calorie labeling takes place and the debate concerning calorie labels reignites, we hope that our research informs the discussion. We agree that the effect of calorie labeling on consumer choices has so far been disappointing. But rather than scrapping calorie counts on restaurant menus altogether, we need to find ways to make this information more effective. Our research suggests one simple change that food retailers can adopt.

In the U.S., calorie labels may have a limited effect when presented to the right of menu items, but they may have a considerable effect when presented to the left. Let’s take a step in that direction.

Steven Dallas, Ph.D., is a behavioral scientist at New York University’s Stern School of Business. Peggy Liu, Ph.D., is an assistant professor of business administration, marketing, and business economics at the University of Pittsburgh’s Katz Graduate School of Business. Peter Ubel, M.D., is a physician, behavioral scientist, and professor of business, public policy, and medicine at Duke University.

One of the most popular New Year’s resolutions is to eat healthier and make better decisions when choosing what foods to eat. But a recent study by Texas A&M AgriLife researchers shows there are often competing and conflicting forces at work when making food choices.

“Consumers who already know the approximate calorie content of various food options and are conscious about calorie intake often choose the lower-calorie food to eat,” said Marco Palma, professor in the Department of Agricultural Economics in the College of Agriculture and Life Sciences and director of the Human Behavior Laboratory at Texas A&M University. “However, when multiple food options are presented together in one menu, the decision to eat healthy may not be as simple as the consumer may expect, depending on the relative calorie distance.”

Palma said the calorie distance — the difference in the number of calories in food items — affects the visceral reaction or gut feeling related to the temptation of food items.

Does a restaurant providing the calorie counts of food items influence your decision making Why or why not?

Marco Palma is a professor in Texas A&M’s Department of Agricultural Economics and director of the Human Behavior Laboratory.


Texas A&M AgriLife

“This relative magnitude difference in food menu options affects a person’s temptation for a food item and ultimately the person’s food choice,” he said. “If a brownie is 500 calories and an apple is 80 calories, then the calorie distance is 420 calories. Similarly, if an orange has 60 calories, then the calorie distance between the apple and the orange is only 20 calories.”

Palma said the presence of a high-calorie item on a menu increases the temptation for it, making low-calorie items less appealing.

The study was recently published in the Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. In their work, the scientists demonstrate how food choices’ calorie distance creates temptation and reduces the benefit of disclosing a food calorie count to the consumer. Authors include Palma; Samir Huseynov, former Texas A&M doctoral student now at Auburn University; and Ghufran Ahmad of the National University of Sciences and Technology in Pakistan.

“Specifically, we researched if calorie labels can lead consumers to choose low-calorie and healthier food decisions,” Palma said. “However, we also observed an opposing factor – temptation – when a higher calorie content serves as a signal for a tastier food product.”

Calorie Labeling Produces Conflicting Results

The overconsumption of unhealthy and high-calorie food has become a public health crisis. In response, food manufacturers and retailers are now legally required to add calorie information to their labels so that consumers can make informed choices regarding calorie intake.

“However, the relevant literature on the effectiveness of offering lower-calorie alternatives has shown mixed results,” Palma said. “Some empirical studies show that calorie labeling decreases calorie intake, while other studies have shown no significant changes or even show an increase in calorie consumption. As a result, the impact of calorie information on calorie intake and any potentially moderating factors has remained an unsolved research question.”

He said the theoretical model the researchers used in the study provides a foundation to explain some of the inconsistencies. It helped identify how temptation is enhanced or reduced depending on the menu consumers see and the relative calorie distance between similar items.

The Temptation Tradeoff

Palma said some recent economic models offer insights into the factors that could potentially alter the impact of calorie information on food consumption.

In such models, a decision-maker derives two kinds of benefits from a choice: normative utility and temptation utility. Normative utility is making self-improving and long-term rational choices, while temptation utility is making choices based on more self-gratifying and short-term impulses.

Researchers model self-control cost as the temptation utility difference between the most and least tempting alternatives on a menu. In other words, as one alternative becomes more tempting, the decision-maker becomes more vulnerable to choosing the high-calorie and more tempting option.

To demonstrate this model, the study uses the example of a soda drinker trying to consume less calories. In one scenario, the person has a choice between a bottle of water and a zero-calorie soft drink. Here, there is a relatively low temptation tradeoff because both options have no calories.

In a contrasting scenario, a higher temptation tradeoff occurs when the person must choose between a bottle of water and a regular calorie soft drink. With this scenario, the choice imposes a higher self-control cost on the decision-maker because a bottle of regular soft drink is typically more tempting to the average soda drinker than a zero-calorie soft drink.

“Generally, choices that are more different from the tempting option are more difficult to accomplish,” Palma said. “For a timely example, overly ambitious New Year’s resolutions to eat healthier typically end in failure because people often take large leaps instead of smaller, more manageable steps. Similarly, radical diet changes can burden the decision-maker with unbearable self-control costs, which in turn can lead to more frequent self-control failures.”

Does a restaurant providing the calorie counts of food items influence your decision making Why or why not?

A choice between two options with a small calorie distance has a low temptation tradeoff.


Chris Hondros/Getty Images

Setting Up The Study

“The identification of the effect of self-control failures on decision-making has always been a challenge,” Palma said. “Additionally, the correlation of visceral feelings with pre-formed habits further complicates the link between temptation and the failure of self-control.”

He said to help disentangle these links, the researchers established a controlled setting in which they could manipulate “temptational” differences in food choice alternatives by using similar items that differed in calorie content.

The study’s model predicted that providing calorie information can draw consumers toward lower-calorie food choices. Also, the researchers considered the competing factors of self-control cost and temptation in their model to show how calorie information can also be ineffective or even backfire.

The researchers conducted two separate experiments — a lab experiment and a lab-in-the-field experiment conducted at a national restaurant chain.

In the lab experiment, decision-makers were given 40 binary-choice menus and were asked to select their preferred snack at the end of the study. The menus each included low- and high-calorie versions of the same snack. For example, subjects were offered an 80-calorie strawberry gelatin and a 10-calorie strawberry gelatin of the same brand.

“This test allowed us to focus on the role of calorie difference between alternatives by controlling product and brand preferences,” Palma said. “We explored the causal relationship between the temptation distance — or calorie distance — and the likelihood of choosing low-calorie snacks.”

The researchers employed a similar design for the restaurant experiment.

“We conducted the second experiment in a restaurant from a national chain using full meals from the restaurant’s menu,” he said. “A ‘no information’ control group received meal descriptions but no calorie information. An ‘accurate information’ group received both meal descriptions and calorie information.”

Subjects made food choices in 86 independent, binary menus and were only allowed to eat the meals inside the restaurant and could not share food with anyone else.

The restaurant experiment allowed the researchers to test their hypotheses with real meals within an actual restaurant setting. Additionally, it allowed them to explore greater relative calorie distances compared to the snacks in the lab experiment. The prices of the meals were identical, and the researchers manipulated the calorie difference by changing the side items.

More Information Helps Make Better Food Decisions

Overall, the study showed providing calorie information increases the likelihood to choose the low-calorie option by anywhere from 3 to 10 percentage points. However, the impact of calorie information depends on the relative magnitudes of the opposing forces behind making food choices.

“Specifically, the study shows there is a significant and positive relationship between the number of calories in snacks and the degree of temptation the snacks generate,” Palma said. “And the effect of calorie information depends on the relative calorie difference. For example, lab experiment subjects were more likely to exhibit self-control and choose low-calorie snacks when they had accurate information about their food choices.”

While the study shows that providing calorie information is important, it also shows that the efficacy of that information depends in large part on menus and menu-dependent temptation feelings and self-control costs. This may also offer a plausible explanation for why calorie labeling laws have not generated the desired outcome of reducing calorie intake.

The study also makes an important contribution by modeling and quantifying menu-dependent self-control and linking the incurred cost to incentivized food choices. This finding is particularly relevant since it may have an impact on how restaurants and other food retailers respond to the new calorie labeling laws by changing the product offerings in their menus.

What effect results from including calorie counts on a restaurant menu?

In conclusion, adding calorie information to menu items highlights the calorie-dense nature of many restaurant meals and increases transparency for consumers. However, additional steps are needed to help individuals make healthier choices.

Do restaurants need calorie counts?

Businesses must also provide, upon request, the following written nutrition information for standard menu items: total calories; total fat; saturated fat; trans fat; cholesterol; sodium; total carbohydrates; sugars; fiber; and protein.

Why is it important to determine the calories of food?

Calorie consumption that is too low or too high will eventually lead to health problems. The number of calories in food tells us how much potential energy they contain. It is not only calories that are important, but also the substance from which the calories are taken.

How do food labels influence your eating decisions?

As we and other colleagues recently reported in the American Journal of Preventive Medicine, food labeling had some effects on consumer choices: They reduced the intake of calories by 6.6 percent, total fat by 10.6 percent, and other generally unhealthy choices by 13 percent.